REVIEW OF RELATED LITERATURE
This chapter shall be discussing the findings of
related researches to this study. It shall provide a discussion on the
significance of this study to the existing literature. The contents of this
portion of the study is gathered and collated from its secondary data.
Students' cognitive performance and
allocation of attention can depend on the degree of consistency between their
past and present experiences in the classroom. Previous findings indicated that
performance change due to evaluation and social comparison manipulations were
simultaneously affected by students' past personal academic experiences. With
the growing diversity among children in today's classrooms, teacher education
programs are increasingly called on to prepare teachers who are able to respond
competently to the demands of inclusive classrooms (Munby & Hutchinson,
1998). General education teachers are largely responsible for meeting the
educational needs of all their students. However, many general educators do not
perceive themselves as adequately prepared to provide a meaningful education to
students with diverse needs (Hinders, 1995; Semmel, Abernathy, Butera, &
Lesar, 1991), Existing preservice teacher education programs may be inadequate
in terms of their abilities to prepare teachers to meet the needs of the
diverse student population they will face in the 21st century (Maheady,
Mallette, & Harper, 1996). Consequently, if teachers are to be successful
in the classrooms of the future, teacher preparation programs must provide
training in the knowledge and skills necessary for working with children with a
wide range of ability levels in the same classroom.
Although there is substantial
evidence that contingent teacher praise, approval, and other forms of positive
reinforcement have positive effects on student behavior and achievement (Alber
& Heward, 2000; Maag, 2001), some researchers have argued against the use
of praise and rewards for student performance (Ryan & Deci, 1996). Kohn,
who has gained considerable notoriety and popularity by giving speeches and
writing papers and books for educators and business managers, has claimed that
the use of "extrinsic motivators" such as incentive plans, grades,
and verbal praise damage the "intrinsic motivation" of students and
employees to learn and work (Kohn, 1993). Kohn has argued passionately and
articulately--but without sound empirical bases--that not only is praise
ineffective, it is actually harmful to students. He has claimed that praise
increases pressure to "live up to" the compliment, insinuates
unrealistic expectations of future success, insidiously manipulates people,
establishes a power imbalance, insults people if awarded for unchallenging
behaviors, and undermines intrinsic motivation.
Conventional wisdom holds that an
extra measure of patience is required to be a good teacher of children with
disabilities. This faulty notion does a great disservice to students with
special needs and to the educators who teach them. Although patience is a
positive and valued trait, in the classroom the idea that teachers must be
patient with special education students often translates into slowed-down
instruction, lowered expectations for performance, fewer opportunities to
respond, and fewer in-class and homework assignments. A related piece of wisdom
goes like this: Students with disabilities can learn, but they learn more
slowly; therefore, they should be given extra time and instruction should be
conducted at a slower pace. Although this reasoning possesses a degree of logic
and common sense, research has found that slowing the pace of instruction makes
things worse, not better, for students with learning problems. To illustrate,
Carnine (1976) conducted an experiment in which instruction was presented to
four first-grade remedial reading students at two paces: slow (intertrial
interval of 5 seconds) and fast (intertrial interval of 1 second or less).
Fast-paced instruction resulted in more learning trials presented by the
teacher, more responses per lesson by the students, better accuracy of student
responses, and better on-task behavior. Systematic replications of this study
have yielded a similar pattern of results (Carnine & Fink, 1978; Darch
& Gersten, 1985; Ernsbarger et al., 2001; Koegel, Dunlap, & Dyer, 1980;
Williams, 1993).
Just as teaching too slowly impedes
learning, teaching with excessive sensitivity to and patience for students with
disabilities may lead to lower expectations, fewer assignments, and students'
participation only when the students "feel like it." Educational
research is unequivocal in its support for the positive relationship between
the amounts of time children spends actively responding to academic tasks and
their subsequent achievement (Brophy & Good, 1986; Fisher & Berliner,
1985; Greenwood, Delquadri, & Hall, 1984; Heward, 1994). When other key
variables are held constant, such as quality of curriculum materials, students'
prerequisite skills, and motivation, a lesson in which students emit many active
responses will produce more learning than will a lesson of equal duration in
which students make few responses (Gardner, Heward, & Grossi, 1994;
Sterling, Barbetta, Heward, & Heron, 1997). Frequent opportunities to
respond, high expectations, and fast-paced instruction are especially important
for students with learning and behavioral problems, because for children who
are behind to catch up, they simply must be taught more in less time. If the
teacher doesn't attempt to teach more in less time the gap in general knowledge
between a normal and handicapped student becomes even greater. (Kame'enui &
Simmons, 1990, p. 11)
Instead of patient teachers, students with
disabilities need teachers who are impatient--impatient with instructional
methods and materials that do not help their students acquire and subsequently
use the knowledge and skills required for successful functioning in school,
home, community, and workplace. Instead of waiting patiently for a student to
learn, attributing lack of progress to some inherent attribute or faulty
process within the child, a teacher should use direct and frequent measures of
the student's performance as the primary guide for modifying instructional
methods and materials to improve effectiveness. Nevertheless, there is widespread
belief in education that creativity is a key to effective teaching. Like
patience, creativity is a desirable and positive characteristic in teachers.
Many thousands of ineffective lessons have been turned into effective ones by
teachers who have creatively adapted instructional materials; developed
prosthetic devices; or changed the mode, form, timing, or other dimension of a
stimulus prompt. The kind of creativity most often implied by this notion,
however, has little to do with systematically monitoring and analyzing a
student's interaction with carefully planned materials and lesson plans to
detect flaws in the instructional design that the teacher might then repair in
some creative fashion (Heward & Dardig, 2001).
It is one thing for a teacher to creatively
design and adapt instructional materials, examples, and procedures to add an
extra degree or two of effectiveness to an already effective set of teaching
skills. It is quite another thing for a teacher to be "creative" in
the absence of a sound curriculum and repertoire of critical instructional
skills. Instead of being told that being creative is the key to good teaching,
teachers should be trained to realize that the first and most important
requisite to effective teaching is obtaining the knowledge and skills necessary
to select and properly use research-based instructional tools (Lovitt, 1996).
Furthermore, teachers often hear that their profession is an art, not a
science, and that not only is it permissible to teach in different ways from time
to time, but such change is good for students. Adding variety to instructional
activities and materials in an attempt to make lessons more interesting and fun
for students is one way in which teachers frequently try to be creative. A
teacher being creative in this way, however, must be careful not to
inadvertently reduce students' opportunities to practice the target skill(s).
Telling teachers they must be
creative may work against the systematic adoption of research-based curriculum
and instructional tools. Since frequently changing methods and materials is a
primary way for teachers to demonstrate their creativity. Some teachers feel
that teaching the same way becomes boring and it is their right to be creative
in the classroom (Purnell & Claycomb, 2001). Teachers are not in the
classroom for their own enjoyment, however, they are in the classroom as
professionals to do a job; children are not in the schools to be pawns for
educators who want to try one unproven method after another because of fad, fashion,
or creative whim (Engelmann, 1992). We may think that unlimited creativity is a
good thing for teachers, but imagine how you would feel if the pilot on your
next flight announced that he wanted to be creative and was going to try a new
idea that he had heard about for landing airplanes. Teacher creativity will
always have an important place in the classroom, but the need and direction for
that creativity should be guided and subsequently evaluated by students'
achievements, not the whims of teachers.
Action Research in the Classroom
Action research is deliberate,
solution-oriented investigation that is group or personally owned and
conducted. Spiraling cycles of problem identification, systematic data
collection, reflection, analysis, data-driven action taken, and, finally,
problem redefinition characterize it. The linking of the terms
"action" and "research" highlights the essential features
of this method: trying out ideas in practice as a means of increasing knowledge
about and/or improving curriculum, teaching, and learning (Kemmis &
McTaggart, 1982). While the concept of action research can be traced back to
the early works of John Dewey in the 1920s and Kurt Lewin in the 1940s, it is
Stephen Corey and others at Teachers College of Columbia University who
introduced the term action research to the educational community in 1949. Corey
(1953) defined action research as the process through which practitioners study
their own practice to solve their personal practical problems. Very often
action research is a collaborative activity where practitioners work together
to help one another design and carry out investigations in their classrooms.
Teacher action research is, according to John Elliott, "concerned with the
everyday practical problems experienced by teachers, rather than the
'theoretical problems' defined by pure researchers within a discipline of
knowledge" (Elliott, cited in Nixon, 1989). Research is designed,
conducted, and implemented by the teachers themselves to improve teaching in
their own classrooms, sometimes becoming a staff development project in which
teachers establish expertise in curriculum development and reflective teaching.
The prevailing focus of teacher
research is to expand the teacher's role as inquirer about teaching and learning
through systematic classroom research (Copper, 1990). The approach is
naturalistic, using participant-observation techniques of ethnographic
research, is generally collaborative, and includes characteristics of case
study methodology (Belanger, 1992). Moreover, action research has been employed
for various purposes: for school-based curriculum development, as a
professional development strategy, in pre-service and graduate courses in
education, and in systems planning and policy development. Some writers
advocate an action research approach for school restructuring. (Holly, 1990; Jacullo-Noto, 1992; Lieberman,
1988; Oja & Smulyan, 1989; Sagor, 1992) Action research can be used as an
evaluative tool, which can assist in self-evaluation whether the
"self" is an individual or an institution.
There is a growing body of evidence
of the positive personal and professional effects that engaging in action
research has on the practitioner (Goswami & Stillman, 1987; Lieberman,
1988). Action research provides teachers with the opportunity to gain knowledge
and skill in research methods and applications and to become more aware of the
options and possibilities for change. Teachers participating in action research
become more critical and reflective about their own practice (Oja & Pine,
1989; Street, 1986). Teachers engaging in action research attend more carefully
to their methods, their perceptions and understandings, and their whole
approach to the teaching process. Lawrence Stenhouse once said, "It is teachers
who, in the end, will change the world of the school by understanding it"
(cited in Rudduck, 1988). As teachers engage in action research they are
increasing their understanding of the schooling process. What they are learning
will have great impact on what happens in classrooms, schools, and districts in
the future. The things teachers learn through the critical inquiry and rigorous
examination of their own practice and their school programs that action
research requires will impact the future directions of staff development
programs, teacher preparation curricula, as well as school improvement
initiatives.
Furthermore, teachers' action
research questions emerge from areas they consider problematic, from
discrepancies between what is intended and what actually occurs. As
Cochran-Smith and Lytle (1990) suggest, the unique feature of teachers'
questions is that they emanate solely neither from theory nor from practice,
but from "critical reflection on the intersection of the two" (p. 6).
Teacher research will force the re-evaluation of current theories and will
significantly influence what is known about teaching, learning, and schooling.
It has been said that teachers often leave a mark on their students, but they
seldom leave a mark on their profession. Through the process and products of
action research teachers will do both. (p.7)
The traditional action research
approach described above has been extended into a form known as
"participatory action research". An important change is the
realignment of the roles of researcher and subject into more collaborative and
synergistic forms. Formerly, responsibility for theorizing rested primarily on
the shoulders of the researcher. In participatory action research, this
responsibility is shared with client participants. In other words, members of
the organization we study are actively engaged in the quest for information and
ideas to guide their future actions. (Whyte et al., 1991, p. 20) This increased
client participation is a major change. The single most distinguishing characteristic
that contrasts participatory action research from earlier forms is the
"co-researcher status" that is accorded to the client participants
(Elden and Chisholm, 1993). Researchers and clients bring their own distinctive
sets of theoretical knowledge into the action research process. Action
researchers bring their knowledge of action research and general information
systems theories. Client participants bring situated, practical theory into the
action research process. As a result, control over the social setting is
realigned. The setting is free to self-reorganize rather than be artificially
determined by the external researchers. In this way, participatory action
research is based on assumptions that reality is situated (Berger and Luckmann,
1966) and social systems are self-referencing (von Foerster, 1984).
Participatory action research can be seen as being founded on more recent
organizational philosophy.
In participatory action research,
it is not necessary for researchers to extensively research theories surrounded
the immediate problem setting in anticipation of action planning. It is assumed
that the researcher cannot acquire the depth of understanding that client
professionals will have already achieved through years of living within the social
context under study. An indirect effect of the full collaboration of all
participants is that participatory action research extends the social scope of
action research. This extension has been noted both in studies beyond the level
of a single production unit or plant, and in studies beyond the Anglo-American
culture (Elden and Chisholm, 1993).
Music as Therapy
Activities and techniques incorporating
music stimuli play potentially rich and varied roles in therapy for persons
with autism. Music therapy techniques can, for example, facilitate and support
the desire to communicate (Thaut, 1984); break patterns of isolation and engage
the individual in external experiences (Baker, 1982; Thaut, 1984); reduce
echolalic responses impeding functional language use (Bruscia, 1982); decrease
stereotyped motility patterns (Scoraci, Deckner, McDaniel, & Blanton,
1982); teach social skills (Reid, Hill, Rawers, & Montegar, 1975); and
facilitate increased language comprehension (Litchman, 1976). Because of
individual differences within the autistic population, no universal rules of
therapy can be applied. While one individual may respond positively to a
certain technique, another might easily be harmed.
Characteristics of impaired
socioemotional functioning can include lack of eye contact, lack of physical
responsiveness, aloofness, lack of peer relations, often obsessive
preoccupation with objects, and maintenance of environmental sameness. While
these may change in intensity as the individual matures, social aloneness
markedly remains (Thaut, 1984). Thaut (1984) further suggests that problems
with social relations are also more amenable to initial therapy than are other
underlying disorders. Autistic persons, especially in the early stages of
relationship building, often physically reject or ignore social contact
attempts by other persons. Music therapy can provide instead an initial object
relation with an instrument. Instead of threatening, the shape, sound and feel
of the instrument will often fascinate the individual. The instrument can thus
serve as an intermediary between client and therapist, providing an initial
point of contact (Thaut, 1984). At the same time, a trained music therapist can
structure this experience from the outset in order to minimize motility rituals
or sensory overload that may draw the individual back into himself or herself.
Listening experiences can provide
additional tactile and visual experience and help to raise awareness of sound
and of another person creating that sound. Music and musical experiences can
provide infinite kinds of relationships which can be the key to successful
therapy with autistic persons. Alvin (1975), in working with autistic children,
was able to draw them slowly outward by using music to develop a series of relationships
between the client and the instrument, the client and the therapist's
instrument, the client and the music, the client and the therapist's music, the
client and therapist, the client and other clients, and so on. Once the barrier
has been interrupted and contact established, the music therapist could pursue
a variety of structured musical experiences that continue to engage these
individuals and draw them further from their internal, ritualistic world. While
the process can be slow and arduous, music therapy provides an unusual and
pleasurable tool that can be easily adapted to meet the changing needs of the
client. As, the individual progresses,
and relationships begin to form music therapy can provide an effective means of
teaching social skins as well. Schmidt, Franklin, & Edwards (1976) found
music to be highly effective in shaping and reinforcing appropriate, social
behaviors. Reid, Hill, Rawer, & Montegar (1975) found music to be
instrumental in teaching social skills which,, in turn, facilitated the
normalization of a child who had previously been isolated from everyday events.
Also significant in music therapy with
autistic persons, is that all of the musical experiences can be structured for
success. Although interactions may be limited by language problems, social
relations can become warm and mutually satisfying if the autistic individual
learns that he or she can succeed in the adapted, therapeutic environment.
Nelson, Anderson, & Gonzales (1984) suggest that, in a sense, the social
disability of autism may be the most treatable part of the disorder, especially
in the context of music therapy, since it depends more on the quality of the
experiences in their environment than on their underlying neuropsychological
characteristics.
Music therapy techniques in the area of
communication attempt to address speech/vocalization production processes and
to stimulate mental processes in respect to conceptualization, symbolization,
and comprehension (Thaut, 1984). On the most basic level, the music therapist
works to facilitate and support the desire or necessity for communication.
Improvised accompaniment to the individual's habitual expressions or behaviors
can demonstrate a communicative relationship between a particular musical sound
and the client's behavior. Autistic persons might perceive such sounds more
easily or readily than verbal approaches, and awareness of the music and of a
relationship between the music and the individual's own actions might serve to
motivate communication (Thaut, 1984).
As the autistic individual begins to
display communicative (verbal or nonverbal) intentions and responses, music can
be used to encourage speech and vocalizations. Alvin (1975) suggests that
learning to play wind instruments is in some ways, equivalent to learning to
make speech vocalizations. It can also strengthen awareness and functional use
of lips, tongue, jaws, and teeth. The use of strong melodic/rhythmic patterns
in. verbal instructions has been found to be beneficial in maintaining better
attention to and comprehension of the spoken word (Thaut, 1984; Mahlberg,
1973). Nelson et al (1984), in a review of the literature, found reports, of
music games being associated with a client's first purposeful speech
production. Litchman (1976) found significant increases in language
comprehension when music was, used in the learning environment. Alvin (1975)
also points out how music can serve as an important link between parent and
child, providing a channel of communication and a model of how both parties can
relate to each other.
Music therapy has also proved useful in
reducing instances of noncommunicative speech patterns, which can impede
progress in learning functional language skills. Bruscia (1982) had dramatic
results when using music therapy in the assessment and treatment of echolalia.
The treatment procedures employed reduced the subjects' echolalia from 95% of
total utterances to fewer than 10% in any setting. Consistent throughout much
of the literature is also the finding that skills and abilities acquired in the
music therapy setting generalize widely across. In autistic persons one sees
constant manifestations of pathological behavior in the perceptual-motor area.
Perceptual and motor disturbances have been linked by a suggested relationship
between motor behavior and the faulty processing of sensory input (Thaut, 1984;
Nelson, et al., 1984). Characteristics of perceptual disturbances frequently
encountered include tactile and kinesthetic receptor preference, hypo- and
hypersensitivity to sensory input such as staring, visual and tactile detail
scrutiny, covering ears etc.
preoccupation with isolated sensory impressions, and avoidance of new
sensory experience. Motor disturbances are often manifested in delayed gross
and fine motor development, poor body awareness/image, self-injury, and
motility disturbances such spinning of self or object, toe walking, rocking,
and/or hand flapping. Music therapy techniques are initially aimed at
decreasing these behaviors, or breaking these stereotyped motility patterns.
Rhythmic activities and movement to music at tempi other than that of body
rocking, for instance, can be helpful in this regard (Thaut, 1984). Soraci,
Deckner, McDaniel, & Blanton (1982) found that music possessing particular
rhythmic characteristics was effective in reducing stereotypic behaviors. When
engaged in stereotypes the individual is effectively "tuned out" from
attending to events in the environment, but, when stereotypes were reduced or
suppressed, the individual could be induced to, engage in productive learning
activities. The music therapist can also structure the musical experience to
ensure that movement responses to music are adaptive and nonrepetitive in
nature (Nelson, et al., 1984).
The autistic individual can begin to
exercise perceptual processes, and learn to relate tactile, visual, and
auditory stimulation through manual exploration of instruments. Movement to
music can also aid in the integration of tactile/kinesthetic and auditory
perception and the differentiation of self/nonself (Thaut, 1984). Action songs
may be beneficial in helping develop auditory-motor coordination and more
refined body awareness/image (Alvin, 1975). Playing with mallets or on a
keyboard can practice functional use of fingers and hands. On a more complex
level, perceptual learning sequences can first isolate, and then combine,
concepts of pitch, loudness, and tempo, by having the client respond in kind on
percussion instruments.
From the most basic level to the most
complex, music therapy techniques can meet the individual at his or her
developmental level, breaking stereotyped behavior patterns and working toward
the integration of different sensory experiences and appropriate motor
responses.
References:
Alber, S. R., & Heward, W. L. (2000). Teaching students to
recruit positive attention: A literature review with recommendations for
practice and future research. Journal of Behavioral Education, 10, 177-204.
Alvin, J. (1975). Music Therapy for the Autistic Child. London:
Oxford University Press.
Baker, B. (1982). The use of music with autistic children. Journal
of Psychosocial Nursing and Mental Health Services, 20(4), 31-34.
Belanger, J. (1992). Teacher as researcher: Roles and expectations.
An annotated bibliography.
Berger, P. and T. Luckmann.
(1966) The Social Construction of Reality: A Treatise in the Sociology of
Knowledge., New York: Anchor Press.
Brophy, J. (1986). Teacher influences on student achievement.
American Psychologist, 41, 1069-1077.
Bruscia, K.E. (1982). Music in the assessment of echolalia. Music
Therapy, 2(l), 25-41.
Carnine, D. (1976). Effects of two teacher presentation rates on
off-task behavior, answering correctly, and participation. Journal of Applied
Behavior Analysis, 9, 199-206.
Carnine, D., & Fink, W. T. (1978). Increasing the rate of
presentation and use of signals in elementary classroom teachers. Journal of
Applied Behavior Analysis, 11, 35-46.
Cochran-Smith, M., & Lytle, S. L. (1990). Research on teaching
and teacher research: The issues that divide. Educational Researcher, 19(2),
2-10.
Copper, L. R. (1990, April). Teachers as researchers: Attitudes,
opinions and perceptions. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American
Educational Research Association. Boston, MA.
Corey, S. (1953). Action research to improve school practice. New
York: Teachers College, Columbia University.
Darch, C., & Gersten, R. (1985). The effects of teacher
presentation rate and praise on LD students' oral reading performance. British
Journal of Psychology, 55, 295-303.
Elden, M. and R. F. Chisholm.
(1993) "Emerging Varieties of Action Research: Introduction to the Special
Issue," Human Relations, (46) 2, pp. 121-142.
Engelmann, S. (1992). War against the schools' academic children
abuse. Portland, OR: Halcyon House.
Ernsbarger, S. C., Tincani, M. J., Harrison, T. J., Frazier-Trotman,
S., Simmons-Reed, E., & Heward, W. L. (2001, May). Slow teacher/fast
teacher: Effects on participation rate, accuracy, and off-task behavior by
pre-K students during small-group language lessons. Paper presented at the 27th
Annual Convention of the Association for Behavior Analysis, New Orleans, LA.
Fisher, C. W., & Berliner, D. C. (Eds.). (1985). Perspectives on
instructional time. New York: Longman.
Gardner III, R., Heward, W. L., & Grossi, T. A. (1994). Effects
of response cards on student participation and academic achievement: A
systematic replication with inner-city students during whole-class science
instruction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 27, 63-71.
Goswami, D., & Stillman, P. R. (Eds.). (1987). Reclaiming the
classroom: Teacher research as an agency for change. Upper Montclair, NJ:
Boynton/Cook.
Greenwood, C. R., Delquadri, J. C., & Hall, R. V. (1984).
Opportunity to respond and student academic achievement. In W. L. Heward, T. E.
Heron, D. S. Hill, & J. Trap-Porter (Eds.), Focus on behavior analysis in
education (pp. 58-88). Columbus, OH: Merrill.
Heward, W. L. (1994). Three "low-tech" strategies for
increasing the frequency of active student response during group instruction.
In R. Gardner III, D. M. Sainato, J. O. Cooper, T. E. Heron, W. L. Heward, J.
Eshleman, & T. A. Grossi (Eds.), Behavior analysis in education: Focus on
measurably superior instruction (pp. 283-320). Monterey, CA: Brooks/ Cole.
Heward, W. L., & Dardig, J. C.
(2001, Spring). What matters most in special education.
Education Connection, 41-44.
Hinders, K. (1995). Dual certification and the regular education
initiative. Journal of Teacher Education, 46, 200-208.
Holly, P., & Southworth, G. (1990). The developing school.
London: The Falmer Press.
Jacullo-Noto, J. (1992, April). Action research and school
restructuring: The lessons learned. Paper presented at the annual meeting of
the American Educational Research Association, San Francisco.
Kame'enui, E. J., Carnine, D. W., Dixon, R. C., Simmons, D. C.,
& Coyne, M. D. (Eds.). (2002). Effective teaching strategies that
accommodate diverse learners (2nd ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ:
Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Kemmis, S., & McTaggart, R. (1982). The action research planner.
Victoria, Australia: Deakin University Press.
Koegel, R. L., Dunlap, G., &
Dyer, K. (1980). Intertrial interval duration and
learning in autistic children. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 13, 91-99.
Kohn, A. (1993). Why incentive plans cannot work. Harvard Business
Review, 71(5), 54-63.
Lieberman, A. (Ed.). (1988). Building a professional culture in
schools. New York: Teachers College Press.
Litchman, M.D. (1977). The use of music in establishing a learning
environment for language instruction with autistic children (Doctoral
dissertation, State University of New York at Buffalo, 1976). University
Microfilms International, No. 773557.
Lovitt, T. C. (1996). What special educators need to know. In W. L. Heward (Ed.),
Exceptional children: An introduction to special education (5th ed., pp.
84-86). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Merrill/Prentice Hall.
Maag, J. W. (2001). Rewarded by punishment: Reflections on the disuse of positive
reinforcement in education. Exceptional Children, 67, 173-186.
Maheady, L., Mallette, B., & Harper, G. F. (1996). The
pair-tutoring program: An early field-based experience to prepare preservice
general educators to work with students with special learning needs. Teacher
Education and Special Education, 19, 277-297.
Mahlberg, M. (1973). Music therapy in the treatment of an autistic
child. Journal of Music Therapy, 10(4),189-193.
Munby, H., & Hutchinson, N. (1998). Using experience to prepare
teachers for inclusive classrooms: Teacher education and the epistemology of
practice. Teacher Education and Special Education, 21, 75-82.
Nelson, D., Anderson, V., & Gonzales, A. (1984). Music
activities as therapy for children with autism and other pervasive
developmental disorders. Journal of Music Therapy, 21.(3),100-116.
Nixon, J. (1989, Winter). The teacher as researcher: Contradictions
and continuities. Peabody Journal of Education, 64(2), 20-32.
Oja, S. N., & Pine, G. J. (1989). Collaborative action research:
Teachers' stages of development and school contexts. Peabody Journal of
Education, 64(2), 96-115.
Oja, S. N., & Smulyan, L. (1989). Collaborative action research:
A developmental approach. Philadelphia: The Falmer Press.
Purnell, S., & Claycomb, C. (2001). Implementing reform: What
Success for All teaches us about including students with disabilities in
comprehensive school restructuring. Alexandria, VA: National Association of
State School Boards of Education.
Reid, D.H., Hill, B.K., Rawers, R.J. & Montegar, C.A. (1975).
The use of contingent music in teaching social skills to a nonverbal
hyperactive boy. Journal of Music Therapy, 12(l), 2-18.
Rudduck, J. (1988). Changing the world of the classroom by
understanding it: A review of some aspects of the work of Lawrence Stenhouse.
Journal of Curriculum and Supervision, 4(1), 30-42.
Ryan, R. M., & Deci, E. L. (1996). When paradigms clash:
Comments on Cameron and Pierce's claim that rewards do not undermine intrinsic
motivation. Review of Educational Research, 66, 33-38.
Sagor, R. D. (1992, April). Collaborative action research: A
cultural mechanism for school development and professional restructuring. Paper
presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research
Association, San Francisco.
Schmidt, D.C., Franklin, R. & Edwards, J.S. (1976). Reinforcement
of autistic children's responses to music. Psychological Reports, 39(2),
571-577.
Semmel, M. I., Abemathy, T. V., Butera, G., & Lesar, S. (1991).
Teacher perceptions of the Regular Education Initiative. Exceptional Children,
58(1), 9-24.
Soraci, S., Deckner, C.W., McDaniel, C. & Blanton, R.L. (1982).
The relationship between rate of rhythmicity and the stereotypic behaviours of
abnormal children. Journal of Music Therapy, 19(l),46-54.
Sterling, R., Barbetta, P. M., Heward, W. L., & Heron, T. E. (1997).
A comparison of active student response and on-task instruction on the
acquisition and maintenance of health facts by fourth grade special education
students. Journal of Behavioral Education, 7, 151-165.
Street, L. (1986). Mathematics, teachers, and an action research
course. In D. Hustler., T. Cassidy, & T. Cuff (Eds.). Action research in
classroom and schools. London: Allen and Unwin.
Thaut, M.H. (1984). A music therapy treatment model for autistic
children. Music Therapy Perspectives, 1(4), 7-13.
von Foerster, H. (1984)
"Principles of Self-Organization -- In a Socio-Managerial Context."
in H. Ulrich and G. J. B. Probst, (eds.) Self-Organization and Management of
Social Systems: Insights, Promises, Doubts and Questions, Berlin:
Springer-Verlag, pp. 2-24.
Whyte, W. F., D. J. Greenwood and
P. Lazes. (1991) "Participatory Action Research: Through Practice to
Science in Social Research." in W. F. Whyte, (ed.) Participatory Action
Research, Newbury Park, CA: Sage, pp. 19-55.
Williams, V. (1993). Effects of two teacher presentation rates
during smallgroup social studies instruction on active student response,
on-task behavior, and academic achievement by elementary students with severe
behavior disorders. Unpublished master's thesis, The Ohio State University,
Columbus.
No comments:
Post a Comment