CHARACTERISTICS OF WILD POLAR BEARS:
Ice – The preferred habitat of the polar
bear is the annual ice that forms next to the shorelines of the continents and
archipelagos throughout the circumpolar Arctic.
Polar bears prefer sea ice
habitat with leads (water channels through ice which may remain
ice-free for only a few minutes to several months, depending upon weather
conditions and water currents.) This is
where they seals, using sea ice as
platform. They also utilize polynyas (areas of open surrounded by ice that remain
open throughout the year due to winds, upwellings, and tidal currents) which
are important breathing and feeding areas for wintering or migrating marine
mammals and birds, making them ideal hunting grounds for the bears.
LAND
– Some polar bears spend part of the year on land. Bears from warmer climates may become
stranded on land. In summer, sea ice
melts along the coastlines, and pack ice (floating sea ice or floes, not connected to
land) moves north. Most pregnant females
spend the autumn and winter in maternity dens.
LONGEVITY – Polar bears are capable of living 20 – 30 years but only
a small percentage of them live past 18.
At the top of the food chain, adult polar bears have no predators;
mortality is mostly due to the hazards of the harsh Arctic environment. However, males occasionally kill other male
competing for mates, and will also periodically kill females protecting
cubs. Cubs less than one year old sometimes
are prey to adult male polar bears and other carnivores such as wolves. Malnourished mothers may also cannibalize
newborn cubs.
HUNTING
Polar bears must
spend at least half their time hunting because usually less than 2% of their
attempts at catching prey are successful.
Even during the spring and early summer, when seals are most abundant
and accessible, a bear catches only every four or five days.
HUNTING
METHODS
Still Hunting – still-hunting is the most
common method of hunting year-round. The
polar bear remains motionless beside a breathing hole or lead edge waiting for
a seal to surface. When it does, the
polar bear bites the head or upper body, and then flips the entire seal onto
the ice. Still-hunting usually takes
less than one hour, but polar bears will wait much longer if necessary.
Stalking on Land
and Aquatic Stalking – this method is used in summer when seals haul out on
sea ice. Once spotted, the seal is
slowly and steadily stalked by the polar bear.
At 50 feet away it will suddenly charge.
With its claws pr teeth the polar bear grabs the seal before it can
leave the ice. Polar bears sometimes
swim underwater to stalk distant seals lying at the edge of an ice floe. When they hunt in this manner they appear to
memorize the route to the seal before slipping quietly into the water. As the bear nears the seal it stealthily
pokes the tip of its nose out to breathe, silently slips backward until it is
submerged, and then swims rapidly underwater until it needs to breathe again.
Stalking birth lairs – stalking ringed seals at their birth
lairs is a hunting method polar bears use in spring when ringed seals give
birth tot heir pups. This method is used
most commonly by polar bear females with cubs less than one year old, for several
reasons: mother seals and pups have the high fat content needed for hungry
polar bear mothers and their growing cubs, male polar bears that may attack
young polar bear cubs don’t normally hunt seals in birth lairs, and the lairs
are usually on sea ice attached to land, allowing young cubs (who have little
protective blubber) to avoid crossing water.
Daily activity
cycle
Polar bears are most active
the first third of the day and least active the final third of the day. . In
the Canadian Arctic, adult female polar bears with cubs hunt about 19% of their
time during the spring and about 38% of their time during the summer. Adult
male polar bears hunt about 25% of their time during the spring and about 40%
of their time during the summer (Stirling, 1978).
When not hunting, polar bears
are often sleeping or resting. a.) On warm days polar bears sprawl out on the
ground or ice, sometimes on their backs with their feet in the air. They may
also make temporary snow or earthen pits to lie in. b.) On cold days polar
bears curl up and often cover their muzzle area. During the winter, some polar
bears excavate temporary dens or find natural shelters to stay warm. They may
use these shelters for several months at a time.
SOCIAL
STRUCTURE
Polar bears are basically
solitary. Usually, only two social units exist: (1) adult females with cubs,
and (2) breeding pairs. That said, polar bears will aggregate (gather in
groups) under several conditions: (1) to feed on large whale carcasses and at
dump sites, (2) in some southern regions, like Hudson Bay, bears aggregate on
land during the ice-free summer and autumn months, and (3) occasionally adult
and sub adult (in the case of polar bears this refers to ages 2½- 5 or 6
years) males will feed and travel together for short periods of time.
Social
behavior
Breeding pairs
remain together for one week or more, mating several times. The most constant
social interaction occurs between mother and cubs. Polar bear mothers are
attentive, frequently touching and grooming their cubs. Cubs remain with their
mothers for 2½years, partly because they need protection from other bears, but
also because they need to be taught how to hunt. The cubs follow their mothers
on the sea ice throughout the year and watch her hunt in all kinds of conditions,
and then they practice hunting themselves. This learning is critical to their
eventual survival. The hardest period of a polar bear's life is the first year
of independence when it can no longer rely on sharing the seals that have been
killed by its mother. Point Defiance's three younger bears, Blizzard, Glacier
and Nanuyaaq, all found themselves reliant on human care before this critical
training period could take place, making them non-releasable.
Aggression
Aggression occurs
between males during the breeding season, and when males attempt to steal food
caught by other polar bears. During the late fall, levels of testosterone (the
male hormone) are lower than at any other time of year, so males fasting along
the coast of Hudson Bay, waiting for freeze-up, show little aggression toward
each other. Nevertheless, males of similar size still undertake extended
ritualized fights to hone their motor skills for when they will need them.
Play
Polar bears
sometimes like to play. Except during periods of very cold weather, bears of
all ages enjoy swimming, especially cubs and sub-adults. As they walk across
the ice, young bears may run towards the water and "belly flop" into
it with a huge splash. They will also pound the ice with their feet in order to
break it. Young cubs chase and tackle their siblings. While with their mother,
and trying repeatedly to copy her behavior, cubs become bored from time to time
and begin playing, but play itself can be extremely important as it helps cubs
develop some of the motor skills they will need in order to survive as adults.
Play fighting has also been observed between aggregating sub-adult and adult
male polar bears.
CHARACTERISTICS OF CAPTIVE
POLAR BEARS
Habitat
and Environment
Generally
speaking, animals housed in a barren environment show an overall decrease in
interaction with the environment. This comes to expression in a variety of
symptoms (Wemelsfelder, 1990). The animals lie down and sleep more, and spend
significantly more time sitting. On the other hand, they over- react to novel
and/or unexpected events with fearful and aggressive responses. Furthermore,
the animals may develop stereotyped patterns of behavior. Such patterns consist
of high repetitive and uniform sequences of behavior, which seem to be of no direct
functional value to the animal. Examples are bar-biting, stereotyped pacing, as
shown by zoo animals such as polar bears and wolves, and various locomotory
stereotypes in laboratory primates. Sometimes such behavior can be damaging to
other animals; licking and nibbling tails and ears of offspring may for example
induce cannibalism.
Feeding
Most captive bears are given
food directly to them. They do not hunt
for prey which makes them idle. Because
of this, they become bored and stereotyped.
In a study conducted in the United Kingdom, it is said that food
presentation has a big impact on the behavior of the polar bears. Location, presentation, and time of food
delivery are important and may also vary.
Occasionally keepers will deposit only the scent of food in an
exhibit. Just as they would in the wild,
exhibit animals may spend hours looking for the origin of the food scent. To enhance randomness and variety where diet
is concerned, sometimes food treats or appropriate live items are placed in the
exhibit. One popular enrichment strategy
is to place food treats inside a puzzle that the animal must manipulate in
order to dispense the food item.
Sometimes, in cases where processed foods are normally fed, exhibit
animals may be given un-processed food or even whole carcasses to feed on. Fur and feathers can be important features
for food handling to an animal. In
carnivores, carcass feeding has been shown to promote the health of the teeth,
gums, and pallet (Ziegler, 1995).
Stress is another factor of concern. It
has been suggested that the stress of life in a cage might contribute to the
development of abnormal behaviors. The inability of the animal to escape from
these experiences might lead to adoption of stereotyped behaviors as a means of
coping with an aversive environment. Some studies have indicated that
stereotypic behavior produces a 'trance-like' state (Stevenson, 1983) and is linked to reduced awareness of the
environment associate stereotypes in human children with analgesia.
Moreover, Wemelsfelder
proposes that in long-term captivity, animal behavior gradually loses its
active and flexible character. In the development of abnormal behavior such as
stereotypes, we see behavior becomes increasingly rigid, and loses its
flexibility. The self-directedness of the behavior shows animals close
themselves off from their environment, rather than interact with it. One may
object and hold that such a process can still be regarded as an adaptation to a
barren environment. The non-adaptive character of abnormal behavior comes,
however, best to expression in the response to novel stimuli or a novel
environment.
POSSIBLE INDICATORS OF DISTRESS IN ANIMALS
Some
possible indicators of distress in animals include 1.) Evidence of physical ill
health, 2.) Evidence of frequent occupational diseases, 3.) Need for the use of
drugs and/or surgery to maintain the system of husbandry and also 4.)
Behavioral changes. Under the fourth
category, such include a.) Performance of abnormal behaviors (that are not normally
in the animals repertoire and which appear to be of little benefit to the
animal: e.g. running at bars, pacing), b.) stereotypes i.e. the performance of
repeated behavior fixed in all details and apparently purposeless (e.g.
crib-biting, wind-sucking, weaving, head twisting) c.) large differences in
time budgets from the wild or feral animal, d.) substantial increases in
behavior related to frustration or conflict (e.g. often behavior relating to
locomotion and/or cutaneous stimulation)
and e.) substantial ontogenic behavioral changes (animals performing behavior
characteristics of a very different time in their development e.g. calves of 16
weeks walking as if they were a day or so old)
The fifth possible indicator is Behavioural
restrictions, which is the inability to perform all the behavior in the
animals’ natural repertoire, which does not cause severe or prolonged suffering
to others.
FIGURE 38
BEARS Maintenance behaviour. |
|
. |
BEARS |
Observed hours |
65 |
Number of individuals |
9 |
. |
|
1) Lying |
18.0 |
2) Sleeping |
3.8 |
3) Standing |
7.9 |
4) Eating |
3.7 |
5) Sitting |
3.7 |
6) Moving |
10.1 |
7) Drink |
1.2 |
8) Hind leg (stand) |
0.2 |
9) Defecate and urinate |
0.1 |
10) Abnormal (stereotypes) |
0.5 |
11) Touch other |
4.0 |
12) Human contact |
16.8 |
13) Pacing |
18.3 |
FIGURE 39
BEARS Other behaviour |
|
. |
BEARS |
Observed hours |
65 |
Number of individuals |
9 |
. |
|
1) Abnormal |
2.3 |
2) Frustration |
2.4 |
3) Social a) affiliative |
11.4 |
b)
aggressive |
0 |
c) vocalise |
0.7 |
4) Object directed |
6 |
5) Self directed |
7.2 |
6) Locomotion a) run |
0.03 |
b)
climb |
0.38 |
The bears displayed no
obvious signs of neurotic or pathological behavior (Figures 38 and 39). Pacing
occurred for 18.3 minutes per hour (30%). This is very high. The bears were not
studied in zoos so we have no comparable figures. Other studies suggest that
this figure may well be in line with the normal figure for zoo bears.
Bears are particularly prone
to developing stereotypes, particularly in zoos. One circus bear had a
stereotypic head twist. This occurred for 0.52 minutes/observed bear hour. No
other stereotypes were observed, although the sample was small.
The bears showed no
aggression either to humans or to other bears during the observational periods.
The bears were active during
the observational periods, spending over 10 minutes in every hour moving around
in their small beast wagons. They lay for 18-minutes/animal hour, but only 3.8
minutes were spent sleeping. They also sat, stood on their hind legs, spent
time touching each other (4 minutes/animal hour) and much time in human contact
(16.8 minutes/animal hour). We have no comparable figures from the wild or zoo
animals at present.
Behavior that might be
associated with frustration or conflict, such as head shaking, head nodding,
running at bars, self-scratching, vocalizing, was relatively common: 2.4
times/bear hour - relatively high compared to some of the other species.
Fear
There was no evidence of fear
(fleeing, freezing) recorded in the bears.
Summary of
distress displayed in bears
Bears did show some evidence
of distress; some stereotypic performance, abnormal pacing and bar biting,
relatively high levels of behavior possibly associated with frustration. They
were active, perhaps abnormally so, but remarkably unaggressive.
REFERENCES:
“Animals in Circuses and Zoos Chaper
4” Available: http://www.polbox.com/k/kiley/kwchapter4.htm
Accessed: April 24, 2003
Derocher, A.E. and I. Stirling. "Observations
of aggregating behaviour in adult male
polar bears (Ursus maritimus)." Canadian
Journal of Zoology 68, 1990,
pp. 1390-1394.
Dalziel, Ian W.D. "Arctic."
The World Book Encyclopedia. Chicago: World Book, Inc., 1994.
Domico, Terry. Bears of the World. New York: Facts On
File, 1988.
“General Information” Available: http://www.pdza.org/document/polarbearinfosheet.pdf
Accessed: April 23, 2003
Harington, Richard C. Denning Habits of the Polar Bear (Ursus
maritimus
Phipps). Ottawa: Canadian Wildlife Service Report
Series Number 5, 1968.
Lyman, C.P. "Hibernation and
Torpor in Mammals and Birds." New York: Academic Press, 1982.
Macdonald, David W., ed. "Encyclopedia
of Mammals". New York: Facts On File Publications,
Inc. 1987.
Nowak, Ronals M., ed. Walker's Mammals of the World.
5th edition. Volume 2. Baltimore:
The Johns Hopkins University Press, 1991.
Parker, Sybil P., ed. Grzimek's Encyclopedia of Mammals.
Volume 3. New York: Mcraw Hill
Publishing Company, 1990.
Priest, Gary “Animal
Management and Enrichment” Available: http://zcog.org/zcog%20frames/Enriquecimiento/englishmanejo/Englis manejo.htm Accessed: April 18, 2003
Sage, Bryan. The Arctic and its Wildlife. New York: Facts On File
Publications, Inc., 1986.
Stevenson, M.F.(1983). The captive environment:
Its Effect on Exploratory and Related
Behavioral Responses in Wild Animals. In Exploration in animals and man. Ed. by J. Aecher &
L.I.A. Birke. pp 176-197
Stirling, Ian, and P.B. Latour. "Comparative
hunting abilities of polar bear cubs of different
ages." Canadian Journal of
Zoology 56, 1978, pp. 1768-1772.
Stirling, Ian. Polar Bears Ann
Arbor: The University of Michigan Press, 1988.
Stirling, Ian, et al. "Habitat
preferences of polar bears in the
western Canadian Arctic in late
winter and spring." Polar Record29 (168), 1993, pp. 13-24.
Wemelsfelder,
Francois (1990) Boredom and Laboratory Animal Welfare. In: Rollin B.E. (Ed),
Wemelsfelder,
Francois “Animal Boredom- A Model of Chronic Suffering Captive Animals and Its Consequences for
Environmental Enrichment” Available:
http://www.psyeta.org/his/vol8/Wemelsfelder.html
Accessed: April
20, 2003
Zeigler, G. (1995) An
Alternative to Processed Meat Diets.
Carcass Feeding at Wildlife
Safari. The Shape of
Enrichment. 4 (1) 1-5
No comments:
Post a Comment