Sunday, December 4, 2022

MANIPULATION OF STEREOTYPIC BEHAVIOR

 


This chapter discusses the stereotypic behavior present in captive animals, mainly polar bears.  It defines stereotypic behavior, its possible causes and methods of behavior manipulation.  It also describes the things that these animals do when bored and its effect on their life.  Finally, it evaluates the mentioned methods and indicates whether the polar bears in which these methods were used actually improved their lifestyle.

What is Stereotypic Behavior?

            Stereotypic behavior has been defined as a repetitive, invariant behavior pattern with no obvious goal or function. A wide range of animals, from canaries to polar bears to humans can exhibit stereotypes. Stereotypic behavior is an abnormal behavior frequently seen in laboratory primates. It is considered an indication of poor psychological well-being in these animals. As it is seen in captive animals but not in wild animals, attention has been focused on the situations in which this behavior develops. (Philbin)

 Possible environmental causes of stereotypic behavior

In the search for causes, the emphasis historically has been placed on an animal's environment. It has been believed that stereotypic behaviors will develop in a deficient environment!. Several aspects of the captive environment of captive animals have been investigated. These include cage size, housing type (individual vs. pairing or groups), stress, and lack of environmental complexity.

Use of such small cages has long been implicated as a cause of stereotypic behavior. The belief is that wild animals confined in small cages will develop abnormal stereotyped behavior? (Broom, Mendl and Zanella, 1995).  The main aim of specification of minimum cage sizes per body weight of animal was to promote more species-specific behavior (including the vertical flight reaction) and to reduce abnormal behaviors.

Stress is another factor of concern. It has been suggested that the stress of life in a cage might contribute to the development of abnormal behaviors. The inability of the animal to escape from these experiences might lead to adoption of stereotyped behaviors as a means of coping with an aversive environment. Some studies have indicated that stereotypic behavior produces a 'trance-like' state (Stevenson,1983) and is linked to reduced awareness of the environment  associate stereotypes in human children with analgesia. Finally, one final aspect of the environment that has been considered to cause stereotypic behavior is boredom. (Philbin)

 

A THEORY OF ANIMAL BOREDOM

Generally speaking, animals housed in a barren environment show an overall decrease in interaction with the environment. This comes to expression in a variety of symptoms (Wemelsfelder, 1990). The animals lie down and sleep more, and spend significantly more time sitting. On the other hand, they over- react to novel and/or unexpected events with fearful and aggressive responses. Furthermore, the animals may develop stereotyped patterns of behavior. Such patterns consist of high repetitive and uniform sequences of behavior which seem to be of no direct functional value to the animal. Examples are bar-biting in tethered sows, stereotyped pacing shown by zoo animals such as polar bears and wolves, and various locomotory stereotypes in laboratory primates. Sometimes such behavior can be damaging to other animals; licking and nibbling tails and ears of offspring may for example induce cannibalism in rats and mice.

As time of confinement proceeds, such patterns tend to become increasingly directed towards the animal's own body or products thereof. Primates may spend long periods of time masturbating, rocking their own body, or eating and regurgitating their own feces. Rats may chase their own tail, tethered sows may show long bouts of chewing air, with no other apparent effect than producing large amounts of saliva. Such tendencies may eventually develop into various forms of compulsive self-mutilation. Laboratory monkeys gnaw at their own limbs or genitals, while parrots will pull out their feathers until completely naked. In summary, the overall decrease in interaction shown by captive animals comes to expression in decrease in behavioral variability and an increase in self directed behaviors (Dantzer, 1986).

The Interpretation of Abnormal Behavior

Behavioral scientists mostly refer to the behavior patterns described in the previous paragraph as "abnormal," because these patterns, by and large, do not occur in the wild. Moreover, they bear a strong resemblance to behavior pathologies in human beings.  It is generally accepted that in human beings, abnormal behavior may be a sign of depression, or other forms of subjective suffering. In animals, however, it is by no means regarded as self-evident that abnormal behavior involves suffering.

Moreover, Wemelsfelder proposes that in long term captivity, animal behavior gradually loses its active and flexible character. In the development of abnormal behavior such as stereotypes, we see behavior becomes increasingly rigid, and loses its flexibility. The self-directedness of the behavior shows animals close themselves off from their environment, rather than interact with it. One may object and hold that such a process can still be regarded as an adaptation to a barren environment. The non-adaptive character of abnormal behavior comes, however, best to expression in the response to novel stimuli or a novel environment.

Various studies indicate that animals who have developed stereotyped behavior patterns no longer actively interact with novel stimuli (Wood-Gush, Stolba and Miller, 1983). Most poignantly, this appears in animals who, after years of close confinement are transferred to a more enriched environment. These animals often fail to pay any form of active attention to the new environment. Instead, they persist in the performance of previous established abnormal patterns. Generally, animals raised in more impoverished environments show less tendency to manipulate novel objects than those raised under enriched conditions; isolated rats, for example, do not move or climb objects, but investigates them in a less active, more generally explorative fashion (such as sniffing) (Einon & Morgan, 1967; Renner & Rosenweig, 1986). Such observations suggest the attention abilities of animals in prolonged captivity may gradually become impaired, and it is meaningless to interpret abnormal behavior as a form of adaptation. To investigate this hypothesis, novel objects should be presented to captive animals over a prolonged period of time and their response toward these objects should be recorded. Such research is presently under way at the Scottish Agricultural College in Edinburg, Scotland. (Wemelsfelder)

 

Polar Bear Behavior

          Polar bears typically experience more problems with confinement than most other animals. Captive polar bears are well known for exhibiting repetitive behavior such as pacing back and forth, particularly when housed in substandard, sensory-deprived exhibits. In fact, pacing is so widespread in captive polar bears that the Dutch verb "to pace" is to ijsberen or "to polar bear." 

(Nielsen, 2003)

The most common of such behaviors is unusual movement, for example, rocking from side to side or pacing, whether it be back and forth or around in circles.  Biting or licking bars or walls also falls under the category of abnormal behavior, as is the most disturbing behavior: self mutilation.  (Lindsay, 2001) They are also extremely intelligent animals. This intelligence has researchers at a dozen zoos across the United States studying polar bear stress. They suspect that the reason captive polar bears show signs of stress more frequently than other animals is primarily because they are so smart. (Associated Press (AP), 2002)

 

PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS: ENVIRONMENTAL ENRICHMENT IN CAPTIVE ANIMALS

Providing enrichments for a large and diverse collection of exotic animals takes both time and financial resources.  At the San Diego Zoo and San Diego Wild Animal Park they have an Animal Behavior Management department that has responsibility for developing enrichment strategies for the animals in their collection.  They also have an enrichment library where, just like the public book library, keepers are free to check out enrichment items that have been pre-approved by the veterinary staff for selected species.  The Behavior Management department stocks and maintains the library.   Items in the enrichment library include; animal scents, extracts, smear foods, hair from other animals, puzzle feeder devices, and a variety of toys.

 

Providing animals the opportunity to make choices and express a wide range of species-typical behaviors is not without its risks.  All enrichment items, whether food or non-food are first  approved by the veterinary staff as it is they  who ultimately must deal with a medical problem should one arise due to an enrichment item.  Food enrichments come from the animal’s regular diet.

 

The original goal for the enrichment library was to create a means for the zoo’s nutritionist and one of the staff veterinarians to review a particular item or strategy and approve it for an entire taxa.  But due to behavioral idiosyncrasies of individual animals or nutritional peculiarities of certain species, attaining the goal of a broad-based taxonomic approval has proved elusive.  In San Diego, the reality is that most enrichments move through the approval process and are approved for a single species or in some extreme cases, for a single animal.  Still they have developed a system that works well and facilitates the enrichment of our animals by their keeper staff.  (Priest)

 

So how can we prevent the development of states such as boredom and depression in captive animals? The notion of animal boredom outlined in this paper suggests successful enrichment should enhance the active character of behavior, resulting in an increasingly frequency of interactive behaviors such as exploration, manipulation, play, and social interaction. At the same time, abnormal patterns of behavior should largely disappear. The question arises whether great effort and expense must be made to achieve these aims, or whether small and relatively simple beginnings can be made. Wemelsfelder  suggests the latter is the case; if they are applied in a way which is meaningful to the animal, simple and inexpensive materials can have far reaching effects on the well-being of animals. (Wemelsfelder)

 

METHODS OF BEHAVIOR MANIPULATION

FOOD AND FOOD PRESENTATION

The presentation and variety of  food items in the animal’s diet is just one of the many strategies employed to add complexity and variability to a captive animal’s life.   Location, presentation, and time of food delivery are important and  may also vary.  Occasionally keepers will deposit only the scent of food in an exhibit.  Just as they would in the wild, exhibit animals may spend hours looking for the origin of the food scent.   To enhance randomness and variety where diet is concerned, sometimes food treats or appropriate live items are placed in the exhibit.  One popular enrichment strategy is to place food treats inside a puzzle that the animal must manipulate in order to dispense the food item.  Sometimes, in cases where processed foods are normally fed, exhibit  animals may be given un-processed food or even whole carcasses to feed on.  Fur and feathers can be important features for food handling to an animal.  In carnivores, carcass feeding has been shown to promote the health of the teeth, gums, and pallet (Ziegler, 1995).  Varied presentation of food items can also be enriching.   Food items are sometimes frozen in large blocks of ice.  Animals such as bears and elephants will investigate and manipulate ice blocks trying to figure out a way to obtain the food item inside the frozen water. (Priest)

 

CHANGES TO THE ENVIRONMENT

As wild animals move through their habitats, the scenery changes.  Environmental enrichment provides changes to the scenery in exhibits.  It  may  include moving the components of the exhibit to different locations in the exhibit, or removing them all together.  Or, it could include adding furniture in the form of  piles of rocks and logs, or digging pits, or placing other novel objects in the animal’s environment.  These objects are deposited in an enclosure and are available to be investigated, explored, and manipulated by the animal.   (Priest)

 

PRESENTATION OF LIVE PREY

Enrichment may also involve the presentation of live prey.  In the United Kingdom, a study has been conducted on visitor attitudes towards the feeding of live prey to exhibit animals (Ings, 1997.)  According to this study, visitors generally felt that “it is natural” and would only object because “it would upset them or their children.”   Similarly, in the United States, public reaction to the feeding of live prey is a concern.  In San Diego, they routinely feed enrichments in the form of live meal worms and crickets with universal public approval.  They have also fed live fish in a variety of public situations including shows, exhibits pools, and exhibits with underwater viewing,  all with little negative public reaction.  The predator species list included;  North American river otter (Lutra canadensis), grizzly bear, (Ursus arctos), polar bear (Ursus maritimus), fishing cat (Prionailurus viverrinus), tiger (Neofelis tigris), gharial (Gavialis gangeticus), and various aquatic turtles.   However, feeding a domestic chick or live rabbit, to the same predator will produce a largely negative and unacceptable public reaction.  Clearly, zoos have some work to do in the area of  public education about the food chain.  The good news for predators is that there has recently been some positive movement in the direction of institutional willingness to feed small, live, warm-blooded prey.  

The Grizzly Discovery Center in Yellowstone National Park, reports having “huge success” in offering the gray wolf (Canis lupis) and the grizzly bear (Ursus arctos horribilis) live rats as an occasional enrichment item (Heine, 1998).

            While the predators are secure in their dens, two to five rats are released in the habitat.  Investigators report that successful hunts have lasted from two minutes to nine hours. About 15% of the time, the prey species does sometimes escape the predator.   To prevent problems that might accompany an overpopulation of  escapees, only male rats are used for release in the exhibits. If the prey escapes the bears or wolves, it is captured if possible and not placed back in the exhibit again.  Grizzly Discovery Center stations interpreters at the exhibit whose job it is to answer questions and  describe the natural behaviors taking place during the enrichment sessions.

 

So, from the public’s perception, there seem to be at least two key variables to being able to successfully offer live, small,  warm-blooded prey as enrichment.  They are: #1 The context in which it occurs, and #2 The visitor’s perception of “fairness” to the prey animal.  Providing the prey species with cover and multiple hiding places extends the value of the enrichment for the predator and may also contribute to the zoo visitor’s sense of “fairness” by making provisions for the fate of the prey in the event it escapes the predator.

 

Offering live enrichments to captive predators is an area that will require some further study and a great deal of sensitivity for the social concerns that may be associated with it.    (Priest)

 

EVALUATION

            With the use of the above-mentioned tactics, it has been observed that captive animals, including polar bears have greatly diminished their stereotypic behavior.  It is because they can work out their hunting skills and are able to roam their confinement more freely, as polar bears are nomadic animals.  Thus, they have more things to do which decreases their boredom.   

 

CONCLUSION

By examining the scientific literature regarding stereotypic behavior in polar bears and other captive animals, I can say that environmental factors such as cage size, housing type, stress and boredom are the primary reasons why they undergo abnormal behavior or stereotype.

When a polar bear has come to this stage, he mostly paces back and forth or around in circles.  He also bites or licks bars or walls and the most disturbing behavior: self mutilation.

However, with the proper behavior manipulation process such as environment enrichment, such behaviors may diminish and give the polar bears a better lifestyle.

 

REFERENCES:

1.    Associated Press (AP) (2002) “Beating Polar Bear Boredom” Available: http://cbsnews.com/stories/2000/01/19/breeds/main151194.shtml  Accessed:  April 19, 2003

2.    Broom, D.M., Mendl, M.T. and Zanella, AJ (1995) “A Comparison of the Welfare of Sow in Different Housing Conditions.”  Animal Science 61 pp. 369-385

3.    Dantzer, R (1986) Behavioral, physiological and Functional Aspects of Stereotyped Behavior:  A Review and Reinterpretation.  Journal of Animal Science pp. 1776-1786

4.    Einon, D & Morgan, M (1976) Habituation of Object contact in Socially Reared & isolated Rats (Rattus Norvegicus).  Animal Behavior 24, pp. 415-420

5.    Heine, J.R. (1998) Enrichment Opportunities in the Greater Yellowstone Ecosystem.  Proceedings of the Third International Conference on Enrichment.  San Diego:  The Shape of Enrichment pp. 112-115

6.    Ings, R, Waran, N.K., Young, R.J (1997) Attitude of 200 visitors to the Idea of Feeding Live prey to 200 Animals.  Zoo Biology.  16: 343-347

7.    Lindsay, Katie (2001) “Captivity” Available:  http://www.english.uwosh.edu/hill/38-385/students/lindsay/website/captivity.htm Accessed: April 18, 2003

8.    Nielsen, Barbara (2003) “Polar Bears International (PBI) Zoo Study”  Available”  http://www.polarbearsalive.org/zoo.php  Accessed:  April19, 2003

9.    Philbin, Nora “Towards an Understanding of Stereotypic Behavior in Laboratory Macaques”  Available: www.awionline.org/Lab_animals/biblio/at-phil.htm Accessed: April 20, 2003

10. Priest, Gary “Animal Management and Enrichment” Available: http://zcog.org/zcog%20frames/Enriquecimiento/englishmanejo/Englishmanejo.htm Accessed:  April 18, 2003

11.  Renner MJ. & Rosenzweig, M.R. (1986) Object Interactions in juvenile rats (Rattus Norvegicus).  Effects of differential Histories. J. Comp. Psychology 100, 229-236

12.  Stevenson, M.F.(1983). The captive environment: Its Effect on Exploratory and Related Behavioral Responses in Wild Animals. In Exploration in animals and man. Ed. by J. Aecher & L.I.A. Birke. pp 176-197

13.  Wemelsfelder, Francois (1990) Boredom and Laboratory Animal Welfare.  In:  Rollin B.E. (Ed),

14.  Wemelsfelder, Francois “Animal Boredom- A Model of Chronic Suffering in Captive Animals and Its Consequences for Environmental Enrichment”  Available: http://www.psyeta.org/his/vol8/Wemelsfelder.html Accessed: April 20, 2003

15.  Wood-Gush, D.G.M., Stolba, A. and Miller, C. (1983) Exploration in Farm Animals and Animal Husbandry.  In: Archer, J & Birke L.I.A.

16.  Zeigler, G. (1995) An Alternative to Processed Meat Diets.  Carcass Feeding at Wildlife Safari.  The Shape of Enrichment.  4 (1) 1-5

 

 

No comments:

Post a Comment